Stages of
Language Acquisition in Children
In nearly
all cases, children's language development follows a predictable sequence.
However, there is a great deal of variation in the age at which children reach
a given milestone. Furthermore, each child's development is usually
characterized by gradual acquisition of particular abilities: thus
"correct" use of English verbal inflection will emerge over a period
of a year or more, starting from a stage where vebal inflections are always
left out, and ending in a stage where they are nearly always used correctly.
There are
also many different ways to characterize the developmental sequence. On the
production side, one way to name the stages is as follows, focusing primarily
on the unfolding of lexical and syntactic knowledge:
Stage
|
Typical age
|
Description
|
Babbling
|
6-8 months
|
Repetitive
CV patterns
|
One-word
stage
(better one-morpheme or one-unit) or holophrastic stage |
9-18
months
|
Single
open-class words or word stems
|
Two-word
stage
|
18-24
months
|
"mini-sentences"
with simple semantic relations
|
Telegraphic
stage
or early multiword stage (better multi-morpheme) |
24-30
months
|
"Telegraphic"
sentence structures of lexical rather than functional or grammatical
morphemes
|
Later
multiword stage
|
30+ months
|
Grammatical
or
functional structures emerge
|
Vocalizations in the first year of life
At birth, the infant vocal tract is in some ways more
like that of an ape than that of an adult human. Compare the diagram of the
infant vocal tract shown on the left to diagrams of adult human
and ape.
In
particular, the tip of the velum reaches or overlaps with the tip of the
epiglottis. As the infant grows, the tract gradually reshapes itself in the
adult pattern.
During the
first two months of life, infant vocalizations are mainly expressions of
discomfort (crying and fussing), along with sounds produced as a by-product of
reflexive or vegetative actions such as coughing, sucking, swallowing and
burping. There are some nonreflexive, nondistress sounds produced with a
lowered velum and a closed or nearly closed mouth, giving the impression of a
syllabic nasal or a nasalized vowel.
During the
period from about 2-4 months, infants begin making "comfort sounds",
typically in response to pleasurable interaction with a caregiver. The earliest
comfort sounds may be grunts or sighs, with later versions being more
vowel-like "coos". The vocal tract is held in a fixed position. Initially
comfort sounds are brief and produced in isolation, but later appear in series
separated by glottal stops. Laughter appears around 4 months.
During the
period from 4-7 months, infants typically engage in "vocal play",
manipulating pitch (to produce "squeals" and "growls"),
loudness (producing "yells"), and also manipulating tract closures to
produce friction noises, nasal murmurs, "raspberries" and
"snorts".
At about
seven months, "canonical babbling" appears: infants start to make
extended sounds that are chopped up rhythmically by oral articulations into
syllable-like sequences, opening and closing their jaws, lips and tongue. The
range of sounds produced are heard as stop-like and glide-like. Fricatives,
affricates and liquids are more rarely heard, and clusters are even rarer.
Vowels tend to be low and open, at least in the beginning.
Repeated
sequences are often produced, such as [bababa] or [nanana], as well as
"variegated" sequences in which the characteristics of the
consonant-like articulations are varied. The variegated sequences are initially
rare and become more common later on.
Both vocal
play and babbling are produced more often in interactions with caregivers, but
infants will also produce them when they are alone.
No other
animal does anything like babbling. It has often been hypothesized that vocal
play and babbling have the function of "practicing" speech-like
gestures, helping the infant to gain control of the motor systems involved, and
to learn the acoustical consequences of different gestures.
One word (holophrastic) stage
At about ten
months, infants start to utter recognizable words. Some word-like vocalizations
that do not correlate well with words in the local language may consistently be
used by particular infants to express particular emotional states: one infant
is reported to have used to express pleasure, and another is said to have used to express "distress or discomfort". For the
most part, recognizable words are used in a context that seems to involve
naming: "duck" while the child hits a toy duck off the edge of the
bath; "sweep" while the child sweeps with a broom; "car"
while the child looks out of the living room window at cars moving on the
street below; "papa" when the child hears the doorbell.
Young
children often use words in ways that are too narrow or too broad:
"bottle" used only for plastic bottles; "teddy" used only
for a particular bear; "dog" used for lambs, cats, and cows as well
as dogs; "kick" used for pushing and for wing-flapping as well as for
kicking. These underextensions and overextensions develop and
change over time in an individual child's usage.
Perception vs. production
Clever
experiments have shown that most infants can give evidence (for instance, by
gaze direction) of understanding some words at the age of 4-9 months, often
even before babbling begins. In fact, the development of phonological abilities
begins even earlier. Newborns can distinguish speech from non-speech, and can
also distinguish among speech sounds (e.g. [t] vs. [d] or [t] vs. [k]); within
a couple of months of birth, infants can distinguish speech in their native
language from speech in other languages.
Early
linguistic interaction with mothers, fathers and other caregivers is almost
certainly important in establishing and consolidating these early abilities,
long before the child is giving any indication of language abilities.
Rate of vocabulary development
In the
beginning, infants add active vocabulary somewhat gradually. Here are measures
of active vocabulary development in two studies. The Nelson study was based on
diaries kept by mothers of all of their children's utterances, while the Fenson
study is based on asking mothers to check words on a list to indicate which
they think their child produces.
Milestone
|
Nelson
1973
(18 children) |
Fenson
1993
(1,789 children) |
10 words
|
15 months
(range 13-19) |
13 months
(range 8-16) |
50 words
|
20 months
(range 14-24) |
17 months
(range 10-24) |
Vocabulary
at 24 months
|
186 words
(range 28-436) |
310 words
(range 41-668) |
There is
often a spurt of vocabulary acquisition during the second year. Early words are
acquired at a rate of 1-3 per week (as measured by production diaries); in many
cases the rate may suddenly increase to 8-10 new words per week, after 40 or so
words have been learned. However, some children show a more steady rate of
acquisition during these early stages. The rate of vocabulary acquisition
definitely does accelerate in the third year and beyond: a plausible estimate
would be an average of 10 words a day during pre-school and elementary school
years.
Perception vs. production again
Benedict
(1979) asked mothers to keep a diary indicating not only what words children
produced, but what words they gave evidence of understanding. Her results
indicate that at the time when children were producing 10 words, they were
estimated to understand 60 words; and there was an average gap of five months
between the time when a child understood 50 words and the time when (s)he
produced 50 words.
All of these
methods (maternal diaries and checklists) probably tend to underestimate the
number of words about young children actually know something, although they
also may overestimate the number of words to which they attribute adult-like
meanings.
Combining words: the emergence of syntax
During the
second year, word combinations begin to appear. Novel combinations (where we
can be sure that the result is not being treated as a single word) appear
sporadically as early as 14 months. At 18 months, 11% of parents say that their
child is often combining words, and 46% say that (s)he is sometimes combining words.
By 25 months, almost all children are sometimes combining words, but about 20%
are still not doing so "often."
Early multi-unit utterances
In some
cases, early multiple-unit utterances can be seen as concatenations of
individual naming actions that might just as well have occured alone:
"mommy" and "hat" might be combined as "mommy
hat"; "shirt" and "wet" might be combined as
"shirt wet". However, these combinations tend to occur in an order
that is appropriate for the language being learned:
- Doggy bark
- Ken water (for "Ken is drinking water")
- Hit doggy
Some
combinations with certain closed-class morphemes begin to occur as well:
"my turn", "in there", etc. However, these are the
closed-class words such as pronouns and prepositions that have semantic content
in their own right that is not too different from that of open-class words. The
more purely grammatical morphemes -- verbal inflections and verbal auxiliaries,
nominal determiners, complementizers etc. -- are typically absent.
Since the
earliest multi-unit utterances are almost always two morphemes long -- two
being the first number after one! -- this period is sometimes called the
"two-word stage". Quite soon, however, children begin sometimes
producing utterances with more than two elements, and it is not clear that the
period in which most utterances have either one or two lexical elements should
really be treated as a separate stage.
In the early
multi-word stage, children who are asked to repeat sentences may simply leave
out the determiners, modals and verbal auxiliaries, verbal inflections, etc.,
and often pronouns as well. The same pattern can be seen in their own
spontaneous utterances:
- "I can see a cow" repeated as "See cow" (Eve at 25 months)
- "The doggy will bite" repeated as "Doggy bite" (Adam at 28 months)
- Kathryn no like celery (Kathryn at 22 months)
- Baby doll ride truck (Allison at 22 months)
- Pig say oink (Claire at 25 months)
- Want lady get chocolate (Daniel at 23 months)
- "Where does Daddy go?" repeated as "Daddy go?" (Daniel at 23 months)
- "Car going?" to mean "Where is the car going?" (Jem at 21 months)
The pattern
of leaving out most grammatical/functional morphemes is called
"telegraphic", and so people also sometimes refer to the early
multi-word stage as the "telegraphic stage".
Acquisition of grammatical elements and the
corresponding structures
At about the
age of two, children first begin to use grammatical elements. In English, this
includes finite auxiliaries ("is", "was"), verbal tense and
agreement affixes ("-ed" and '-s'), nominative pronouns
("I", "she"), complementizers ("that",
"where"), and determiners ("the", "a"). The
process is usually a somewhat gradual one, in which the more telegraphic
patterns alternate with adult or adult-like forms, sometimes in adjacent utterances:
- She's gone. Her gone school. (Domenico at 24 months)
- He's kicking a beach ball. Her climbing up the ladder there. (Jem at 24 months).
- I teasing Mummy. I'm teasing Mummy. (Holly at 24 months)
- I having this. I'm having 'nana. (Olivia at 27 months).
- I'm having this little one. Me'll have that. (Betty at 30 months).
- Mummy haven't finished yet, has she? (Olivia at 36 months).
Over a year
to a year and a half, sentences get longer, grammatical elements are less often
omitted and less often inserted incorrectly, and multiple-clause sentences
become commoner.
Perception vs. production again
Several
studies have shown that children who regularly omit grammatical elements in
their speech, nevertheless expect these elements in what they hear from adults,
in the sense that their sentence comprehension suffers if the grammatical
elements are missing or absent.
Progress backwards
Often
morphological inflections include a regular case ("walk/walked",
"open/opened") and some irregular or exceptional cases
("go/went", "throw/threw", "hold/held"). In the
beginning, such words will be used in their root form. As inflections first
start being added, both regular and irregular patterns are found. At a certain
point, it is common for children to over-generalize the regular case, producing
forms like "bringed", "goed"; "foots",
"mouses", etc. At this stage, the child's speech may actually become
less correct by adult standards than it was earlier, because of
over-regularization.
This
over-regularization, like most other aspects of children's developing grammar,
is typically resistant to correction:
CHILD: My teacher holded the baby
rabbits and we patted them.
ADULT: Did you say your teacher held
the baby rabbits.
CHILD: Yes.
ADULT: What did you say she did?
CHILD: She holded the baby rabbits
and we patted them.
ADULT: Did you say she held them
tightly?
CHILD: No, she holded them loosely.
Tidak ada komentar:
Posting Komentar